Statistical Consultation Line: (865) 742-7731
Accredited Professional Statistician For Hire
  • Contact Form

Causality in Statistical Power: Isomorphic Properties of Measurement, Research Design, Effect Size, and Sample Size

3/18/2016

1 Comment

 

Newest publication from Dr. Eric Heidel, creator of Research Engineer

Directly related to Statistical Power engine of Research Engineer

My newest published article in Scientifica is now available for download online and on the Research Engineer website. The creation of the Statistical Power engine of Research Engineer led me to write the article. Click on the Download Article button below to download a .pdf directly from the website or click on the Statistical Power button to be taken to the aforementioned engine. Many thanks and regards to everyone that uses Research Engineer! -EH

Download Article
Statistical Power
Research Engineer Home Page
Scale, LLC
1 Comment

Within-subjects designs increase statistical power

10/4/2015

1 Comment

 

Within-subjects designs increase statistical power

Each participant serves as their own control in within-subjects designs

Within-subjects designs increase statistical power. because participants serve as their own control. Between-subjects designs necessitate more observations of the outcome to be able to effectively compare independent groups on an outcome. Multivariate analyses further decrease statistical power in that many more observations of the outcome to detect significant effects. At least 20 -40 more observations of the outcome have to collected per variable entered into a simultaneous of hierarchial regression model in order to meet statistical power when trying to account for demographic, etiological, clinical, and confounding effects.

Within-subjects designs, when coupled with with continuous outcomes, large effect sizes, limited variance in the outcome and a large sample size, greatly increase statistical power. Small effect sizes are also easier to detect using within-subjects statistics because participants serve as their own control. Within-subjects design also provide more statistical power when small sample sizes are used.    

Scale, LLC
1 Comment

Case series are the lowest level of clinical evidence

1/28/2015

1 Comment

 

Case series are used to study rare outcomes and generate hypotheses

Yield measures of effect size and test methodologies

Case series designs yield the lowest form of observational evidence. Researchers choose a series of cases in the population that share some sort of similar characteristic and then they analyze pertinent predictor, demographic, and clinical factors associated with the outcome in the group of cases.  Case series designs are at times also called pilot studies.  

Case series designs are often employed in basic science and pre-clinical research.  They are useful for generating hypotheses, effect sizes for future power analyses, and studying extremely rare outcomes.  

Case series designs are an excellent choice for novice researchers looking to get their feet "wet" in empirical pursuits. They also prove their worth when evidence-based measures of effect do not exist in the literature.  Running a small pilot study or case series can yield important measures of effect. 

Scale, LLC
1 Comment

Small sample sizes, Type II errors, and empirical reasoning

11/18/2014

0 Comments

 

Small sample sizes can lead to Type II errors

Significant effects may not be able to be detected

In instances where a phenomenon or outcome is less prevalent in the population, scientists are forced to work small sample sizes. It is just the nature of the science, and the phenomenon or outcome.

1. When working with smaller sample sizes, adequate statistical power (and therefore statistical significance) is VERY hard to achieve.

2. There is limited precision and accuracy when using categorical or ordinal outcomes, which can further decreases statistical power.

3. When measuring for small effect sizes, small sample sizes cannot provide enough variance in the outcome to detect clinically meaningful, but small effects. This REALLY decreases your statistical power since inferential statistics depend upon variance in the mathematical sense.

With this being said, remember to interpret the p-values yielded from RCT level studies with small sample sizes in the context of the aforementioned points. If a treatment effect does not obtain statistical significance, but appears to be CLINICALLY SIGNIFICANT with a p-value approaching significance (Type II error), then perhaps more credence can be found in the effect.

If researchers run bivariate tests on 30 different outcomes with less than 20 observations and claim significance without a Bonferroni adjustment, throw the article out.

Scale, LLC
0 Comments

Mastery of the literature

11/6/2014

0 Comments

 

Mastery of the literature leads to relevant research questions

Become an expert in the empirical field of endeavor

There is nothing more important when designing and conducting research than being heavily vested in the associated knowledge base. Research questions are born and formulated out of the literature. One cannot argue for a "gap" in the literature unless he or she has put forth the time and effort to know all of the literature. The literature also makes it very easy to make hard decisions in the preliminary phases of study planning.

Here is what the literature can do for you:

1. Give you an evidence-based measure of effect to use in an a priori power analysis. It will show more empirical rigor on your part if you use the values from the most current and highest-quality evidence available.

2. Help you choose the "gold standard" outcome that is most generalizable and applicable to your audience and peers. Using the best outcome measure available increases the internal validity of your study as well. If the same outcome is used in many studies, then it has more validity evidence to back it up. This, again, shows stronger empirical reasoning on your part.

3. Allow you to ask a question that is relevant and that will generate new knowledge. You will be able to pass the "So what?" question with ease when you know the literature. You will know what new knowledge needs to be generated and how it is relevant in the context of the existing literature.

4. Help you choose the correct research design to answer your research question. If you find that the literature only has observational evidence related to your area of interest, then you can make the informed decision to employ a more complex design to yield causal effects.   

Scale, LLC
0 Comments

Feasible research questions are answerable

10/31/2014

0 Comments

 

Feasible research in terms of scope, time, resources, and expertise

Changing the face of medicine versus completing a research study

I have conducted thousands of statistical consultations over the years and have worked with many novice resident researchers over that time. One cannot help but admire the spirit, energy, and motivation of young people wanting to make an impact on medicine through research. I enjoy the zeal and drive of bright people wanting to be physicians and researchers. This is a good thing!

That being said, I spend a lot of my time with novice researchers using deductive reasoning to hone down their research questions into something tangible and feasible. They come into the office with an idea that will change medicine forever and we will be cruising around the Caribbean in a year! This has never been researched before!  No one has ever done this before! Trust me, I want all of these proclamations to be true and I also want to change the face of medicine. Yet, most times it just not feasible to do so given the time, resources, participants, competencies and environment associated with the study.

I focus on a few primary areas when it comes to feasible research questions with my consultees:

1. Participant pool - Are there enough participants available in the immediate clinical or empirical environment to achieve adequate statistical power for inferential analyses? How will you recruit the participants? What are your inclusion and exclusion criteria? Inclusion and exclusion criteria may need to be modified to increase sample size.

2. Effect size - Small effect sizes require large sample sizes.    

3. Research design - Retrospective designs are always more feasible because the data already exists.

4. Communication - Research never occurs in isolation. Researchers should communicate and collaborate with their peers regarding their research projects. Attendings and academic physicians can give you ideas on how to feasibly conduct your research.

5. Time - What is the time frame for the study from inception to publication? How much time do you have to set aside for the research study? Does the completion of your research coincide with abstract deadlines of interest?

6. Power analysis - Conduct an a priori power anlaysis based on an evidence-based measure of effect to see if the study is feasible in regards to sample size needed to achieve power.

Scale, LLC
0 Comments

Values needed for sample size calculations

10/8/2014

0 Comments

 

Evidence-based measures of effect

Use the empirical literature to your advantage

One of the most important things you can do when designing your study is to conduct an a priori power analysis. Doing so will tell you how many people that you will need in your sample size to detect the effect size or treatment effect in your study.

Without an a priori calculation, you could frivolously waste months or years of your life conducting a study only to find out that you only needed 100 in each group to achieve significance. Or, with the inverse, you conduct a study with only 50 patients and find out in a post hoc fashion that you would have needed 10,000 to prove your effect!  

If you are using Research Engineer and G*Power to run your analyses, here are the things you will need:

1. An evidence-based measure of effect from the literature is the first thing you should seek out. Find a study that is theoretically, conceptually, or clinically similar to your own. Try to find a study that uses the same outcome you plan to use in your study.  

2. Use the means, standard deviations, and proportions from these published studies as evidence-based measures of effect size to calculate how large of a sample size you will need. These values will be reported in body of the results section or in tables within the manuscript. It shows more empirical rigor on your part if you conduct an a priori power analysis based on a well-known study in the field.

3. Plug these values into G*Power using the steps published on the sample size page to find out how many people you will need to collect for your study.

Scale, LLC
0 Comments

G*Power for the masses

9/26/2014

0 Comments

 

G*Power is a necessary tool for every researcher's toolkit

Easy statistical power and sample size calculations

I'm trying to run an online business so I'm fully Google-integrated. I see that there many search queries of different derivations related to sample size calculation as it relates to behind-the-scenes tracking measures.

There is an open-source tool available to EVERYONE that allows you to calculate your own a priori and post hoc power analyses. It is called G*Power and as your personal statistical consultant, I highly suggest you go to the following web address and download Version 3.0 to your respective device:

http://www.gpower.hhu.de/en.html    

The researchers that developed this program have made a great contribution to science. It is truly a great and FREE program that can run a litany of different power analyses. You can find out in minutes how large of a sample size that you need, given that you have an idea of the effect size that you are attempting to detect in your study.

Use means, proportions, and variance measures from published studies in your field to have the most empirically rigorous hypothesized effect. Enter these values into G*Power and the adjust the variance and magnitude of the effect size to see how the required sample size changes.   

Click on the Sample Size button to access the methods of conducting and interpreting sample size calculations for ten different statistical tests.

Sample Size
Scale, LLC
0 Comments

Effect size, sample size, and statistical power

9/15/2014

0 Comments

 

Effect size, sample size, and statistical power

Choose an effect size to maximize statistical power and decrease sample size

Effect size, sample size, and statistical power are nebulous empirical constructs that require a strong working knowledge of each in a conceptual fashion.  Also, there are basic interdependent relationships that exist amongst the three constructs. A change in one will ALWAYS exact a predictable and static change in the other two.

An effect size is the hypothesized difference expected by researchers in an a priori fashion between independent groups (between-subjects analysis), across time or observations (within-subjects analysis), or the magnitude and direction of association between constructs (correlations and multivariate analyses).

Effect size planning is perhaps the HARDEST part of designing a research study. Oftentimes, researchers have NO IDEA of what type of effect size they are trying to detect.

First and foremost, when researchers cannot state the hypothesized differences in their outcomes, an evidence-based measure of effect yielded from a published study that is theoretically or conceptually similar to the phenomenon of interest should be used. Using an evidence-based measure of effect in an a priori power analysis shows more empirical rigor on the part of the researchers and increases the internal validity of the study with the use of published values.

Sample size is the absolute number of participants that are sampled from a given population for purposes of running inferential statistics. The nomenclature of the word, inferential, denotes the basic empirical reasoning that we are drawing a representative sample from a population and then conducting statistics in order to make inferences back to said population. An important part of preliminary study planning is to specify the inclusion and exclusion criteria for participation in your study and then getting an idea of how large a participant pool you have available to you from which to draw a sample for purposes of running inferential statistics.

Due to the underlying algebra associated with mathematical science, large sample sizes will drastically increase your chances of detecting a statistically significant finding, or in other terms, drastically increase your statistical power. Large sample sizes will also allow you to detect both large and small effect sizes, regardless of scale of measurement of the outcome, research design, and/or magnitude, variance, and direction of the effect. Small sample sizes will decrease your chances of detecting statistically significant differences (statistical power), especially with categorical and ordinal outcomes, between-subjects and multivariate designs, and small effect sizes.

Statistical power is the chance you have as a researcher to reject the null hypothesis, given that the treatment effect actually exists in the population. Basically, statistical power is the chance you have of finding a significant difference or main effect when running statistical analyses.  Statistical power is what you are interested in when you ask, "How many people do I need to find significance?"

In the applied empirical sense, measuring for large effect sizes increases statistical power. Trying to detect small effect sizes will decrease your statistical power. Continuous outcomes increase statistical power because of increased precision and accuracy in measurement. Categorical and ordinal outcomes decrease statistical power because of decreased variance and objectivity of measurement. Within-subjects designs generate more statistical power due to participants serving as their own controls. Between-subjects and multivariate designs require more observations to detect differences and therefore decrease statistical power.      

Scale, LLC
0 Comments

    Archives

    March 2016
    January 2016
    November 2015
    October 2015
    September 2015
    August 2015
    July 2015
    May 2015
    April 2015
    March 2015
    February 2015
    January 2015
    December 2014
    November 2014
    October 2014
    September 2014

    Author

    Eric Heidel, Ph.D. is Owner and Operator of Scalë, LLC.

    Categories

    All
    95% Confidence Interval
    Absolute Risk Reduction
    Accuracy
    Acquiring Clinical Evidence
    Adjusted Odds Ratio
    Affordable Care Act
    Alpha Value
    ANCOVA Test
    ANOVA Test
    Applying Clinical Evidence
    Appraisal Of The Literature
    Appraising Clinical Evidence
    A Priori
    Area Under The Curve
    Asking Clinical Questions
    Assessing Clinical Practice
    AUC
    Basic Science
    Beta Value
    Between-subjects
    Biserial
    Blinding
    Bloom's Taxonomy
    Bonferroni
    Boolean Operators
    Calculator
    Case-control Design
    Case Series
    Categorical
    Causal Effects
    Chi-square
    Chi-square Assumption
    Chi-square Goodness-of-fit
    Classical Test Theory
    Clinical Pathways
    Clustered Random Sampling
    Cochran-Mantel-Haenszel
    Cochran's Q Test
    Coefficient Of Determination
    Cognitive Dissonance
    Cohort
    Comparative Effectiveness Research
    Comparator
    Concurrent Validity
    Confidence Interval
    Confirmatory Factor Analysis
    Construct Specification
    Construct Validity
    Continuous
    Control Event Rate
    Convenience Sampling Method
    Convergent Validity
    Copyright
    Correlations
    Count Variables
    Cox Regression
    Cronbach's Alpha
    Cross-sectional
    Curriculum Vitae
    Database Management
    Diagnostic Testing
    EBM
    Education
    Effect Size
    Empirical Literature
    Epidemiology
    Equivalency Trial
    Eric Heidel
    Evidence-based Medicine
    Exclusion Criteria
    Experimental Designs
    Experimental Event Rate
    Facebook
    Factorial ANOVA
    Feasible Research Questions
    FINER
    Fisher's Exact Tests
    Friedman's ANOVA
    Generalized Estimating Equations (GEE)
    "gold Standard" Outcome
    G*Power
    Guidelines For Authors
    Hazard Ratio
    Hierarchical Regression
    Homogeneity Of Variance
    Hypothesis Testing
    ICC
    Incidence
    Inclusion Criteria
    Independence Of Observations Assumption
    Independent Samples T-test
    Intention-to-treat
    Internal Consistency Reliability
    Interquartile Range
    Inter-rater Reliability
    Interval Variables
    Intervention
    Intraclass Correlation Coefficient
    Isomorphism
    Item Response Theory
    Kaplan-Meier Curve
    Kappa Statistic
    KR-20
    Kruskal-Wallis
    Kurtosis
    Levene's Test
    Likert Scales
    Linearity
    Listwise Deletion
    Logarithmic Transformations
    Logistic Regression
    Log-Rank Test
    Longitudinal Data
    MANCOVA
    Mann-Whitney U
    MANOVA
    Mass Emails In Survey Research
    Math
    Mauchly's Test
    McNemar's Test
    Mean
    Measurement
    Median
    Medicine
    Merging Databases
    Missing Data
    Mode
    Multinomial Logistic Regression
    Multiple Regression
    Multivariate Statistics
    Negative Binomial Regression
    Negative Predictive Value
    Nominal Variables
    Nonequivalent Control Group Design
    Non-inferiority
    Non-inferiority Trial
    Non-parametric Statistics
    Non-probability Sampling
    Normality
    Normality Of Difference Scores
    Normal Probability Plot
    Novel Research Question
    Number Needed To Treat
    Observational Research
    Odds Ratio With 95% CI
    One-sample Median Tests
    One-sample T-test
    One-sided Hypothesis
    One-Way Random
    Operationalization
    Ordinal
    Outcome
    Outliers
    Parametric Statistics
    Pearson's R
    Ph.D.
    Phi Coefficient
    PICO
    Pilot Study
    Point Biserial
    Poisson Regression
    Population
    Positive Predictive Value
    Post Hoc
    Post-positivism
    PPACA
    PPV
    Precision
    Predictive Validity
    Prevalence
    Principal Components Analysis
    Probability Sampling
    Propensity Score Matching
    Proportion
    Proportional Odds Regression
    Prospective Cohort
    Psychometrics
    Psychometric Tests
    Publication
    Publication Bias
    Purposive Sampling
    P-value
    Random Assignment
    Randomized Controlled Trial
    Random Selection
    Rank Biserial
    Ratio Variables
    Receiver Operator Characteristic
    Regression
    Regression Analysis
    Relative Risk
    Relevant Research Question
    Reliability
    Repeated-measures ANOVA
    Repeated-measures T-test
    Research
    Research Design
    Research Engineer
    Research Journal
    Research Question
    Residual Analysis
    Retrospective Cohort
    ROC Curve
    Sample Size
    Sampling
    Sampling Error
    Sampling Method
    Scales Of Measurement
    Science
    Search Engine
    Search Query
    Sensitivity
    Simple Random Sampling
    Sitemap
    Skewness
    Social Science
    Spearman-Brown
    Spearman's Rho
    Specificity
    Specificity In Literature Searching
    Sphericity Assumption
    Split-half Reliability
    SPSS
    Standard Deviation
    Standards Of Care
    Statistical Analysis
    Statistical Assumptions
    Statistical Consultation
    Statistical Power
    Statistical Power Analysis
    Statistical-power-test
    Statistician
    Statistics
    Stratified Random Sampling
    Survey
    Survey Construct Specification
    Survey Methods
    Systematic Review
    Test-Retest Reliability
    Twitter
    Two-sided Hypothesis
    Two-Way Mixed
    Two-Way Random
    Type I Error
    Type II Error
    Unadjusted Odds Ratio
    Validity
    Variables
    Variance
    Wilcoxon
    Within-subjects
    YouTube


    Contact Form

Contact Dr. Eric Heidel
consultation@scalelive.com
(865) 742-7731

Copyright © 2022 Scalë. All Rights Reserved. Patent Pending.